Original

Influence of cognitive impairment on the freezing of gait in non demented people with Parkinson’s disease

J.M. Cancela-Carral, C.M. Nascimento, S. Varela, M. Seijo-Martínez, L. Lorenzo-López, J.C. Millán-Calenti, C. Domínguez-Vivero, C. Ayán [REV NEUROL 2018;66:289-296] PMID: 29696615 DOI: https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.6609.2017289 OPEN ACCESS
Volumen 66 | Number 09 | Nº of views of the article 18.780 | Nº of PDF downloads 945 | Article publication date 01/05/2018
Icono-PDF-OFF Download PDF Castellano Icono-PDF-OFF Download PDF English Citation Search in PubMed
Share in: Facebook Twitter
Go to another issue
ABSTRACT Artículo en español English version
INTRODUCTION Freezing of gait (FOG) is a motor disturbance usually appearing in advanced Parkinson’s disease (PD). Cognitive and executive function seems to play an important role in this phenomenon.

AIM To investigate if cognitive and kinematic parameters correlate with FOG in PD patients without dementia.

PATIENTS AND METHODS We conducted an observational cross-sectional study. Participants were classified in two groups: freezers and non-freezers. Clinical information was obtained by Hoehn & Yahr scale, Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale and balance test of Short Physical Performance Battery. Cognitive function was evaluated using Minimental Examination and the Fuld Object Memory Evaluation; executive function was assessed with the Frontal Assessment Battery test. Battery kinematic parameters were assessed by means of gait speed, cadence, stride length and stride time.

RESULTS Twenty-five participants with PD without dementia completed the evaluation. Statistical significant differences between freezers and non-freezers were found in global cognition (p = 0.02), memory (p = 0.04), executive function (p = 0.04), cadence (p = 0.02), stride length (p = 0.04) and stride time (p = 0.01). CONCLUSION. Cognitive parameters may have an important contribution to the manifestation of freezing of gait in PD. These results may have important clinical implications for developing future non-pharmacological and cognitive interventions strategies targeted to PD patients with FOG.
KeywordsCognitionGaitMotor disturbanceOlder peopleParkinson’s disease CategoriesDemenciaNeurodegeneraciónNeuropsicologíaNeuropsiquiatríaTrastornos del movimiento
FULL TEXT Artículo en español English version

Introduction


Freezing of gait (FOG) is a complex and disabling episodic motor phenomenon usually appearing in advanced Parkinson’s disease (PD) [1]. FOG consists of an inability to generate steps impairing forward gait that makes the patient remains, literally, glued to the floor [2]. It usually occurs in specific situations such as making turns or walking through a door. FOG does not respond well to dopaminergic medication [3]. It is a definite risk factor for falls and its appearance marks a downturn in the disease course of individuals with PD [4].

The mechanisms underlying this phenomenon are largely unknown and various hypotheses attribute FOG to abnormal gait pattern generation, problems with central drive and automaticity of movement, abnormal coupling of posture with gait, perceptual malfunction and frontal executive dysfunction [5].

FOG may be, in part, a result of dopaminergic down-regulation. Motor disturbances related to gait akinesia seem to be linked to low dopamine striatum uptake [1] and movement initiation can be interfered by striatal dopamine receptor blockade [6]. FOG may be, in part, a result of dopaminergic down-regulation. However, the appearance of this gait disturbance during the parkinsonian on-state and its poor response to levodopa [3] implicates other non-motor and non-dopaminergic factors. Cholinergic areas, including the pedunculopontine nucleus [7] may also be involved in FOG pathophysiology as pharmacological central cholinergic potentiation with antidementia drug rivastigmine lowers the risk of falls. Noradrenergic therapy has also proved to be useful in some PD patients. Increasing evidence recognizes that cognitive and executive function have a center role in the FOG phenomenon [8]. Tasks demanding complex gait adaptations can be compromised if the executive control system is impaired [9]. Moreover, gait anticipatory mechanisms and motor strategic planning are involved since freezing episodes frequently occur in situations when the patient turns, adjusts his gait to a pattern on a crowded area or when a change in gait is prompted by crossing a door or obstacle [10]. Upon an upcoming event, patients with FOG frequently experience impairment and interruptions on their planned movement sequence [11]. FOG episodes may benefit from external sensory cues suggesting that sensory and perceptual pathways are also involved [12,13].

Considering the role of frontal-lobe functions in gait and complex motor behaviors we hypothesize that there is an intrinsic relationship between cognitive impairment in executive function and abnormal posture and gait related to FOG. The aim of this study is to compare kinematic parameters, executive function and cognitive performance in a group of non-demented PD patients with and without FOG.
 

Patients and methods


Participants


Patients with PD were recruited through personal letters from the Asociación de Párkinson Galicia-Bueu using the following inclusion criteria: PD according to the diagnostic criteria of the United Kingdom Parkinson’s Disease Society Brain Bank [14]; stages 1-3 on the Hoehn and Yahr (H&Y) scale [15]; and stable doses of antiparkinsonian medication (dopaminergic medication dosage not changed at least one month prior to assessment).

Exclusion criteria were: dementia (DSM-IV criteria); other major neurological disorders, neuropsychiatric comorbidity or acute illness limiting the evaluation protocol; and refusal to participate by the patient or his/her caregiver.

The ethics committee (CEIC 2011/343) approved the study and all participants and/or caregiver gave informed written consent.

Outcome measures


The medical staff of the association was previously trained for all assessment tools and performed the clinical evaluation. The participants were tested approximately one hour after the last dose of anti-parkinsonian drug. All were in the ‘on’ phase.

Clinical parameters
 
  • Demographic information including age, sex, academic level and medical history of each patient were gathered with the database form created for this purpose.
  • Clinical staging of PD was measured using the H&Y scale [15].
  • Patients’ motor condition and disease severity were measured using the Spanish validated version of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) [16,17] in order to assess functional status (subsection II) and motor function (subsection III).
  • Postural control was assessed by means of the balance test of the Short Physical Performance Battery (SPPB) [18].

Cognitive parameters
 
  • Global cognition was evaluated by a personal interview of the patient and the Spanish-adapted version of the Mini Mental Status Examination (MEC) [19].
  • Frontal cognitive functions were assessed by means of the Spanish validated version of the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) [20,21].
  • Memory: the Fuld Object Memory Evaluation (FOME) [22] was used to assess immediate and delayed memory function [23]. Two FOME scores namely total storage (range: 0-50) and delayed recall (range: 0-10) were derived to assess encoding and retrieval function respectively, with lower scores indicating more impairment.

Kinematic assessment

Gait speed (m/s), cadence (steps/min), stride length (m), stride time (s), rate single support/double-support were calculated after counting steps and time needed for a 10m walk, turn and walk at the same route back at the patients’ preferred speed. All evaluations were recorded with an automatic computerized video motion analysis system (Sports Motion-Pro Trainer DV Motion Analysis). This device allows for a biomechanical gait analysis through of recording spatiotemporal and sagittal plane kinetic and kinematics by means of a video motion system. To this aim, spherical retro-reflective markers were placed on specific anatomical points of the participants’ right lower limbs, enabling three-dimensional analysis during the gait cycle.

Design and data collection protocol


This observational cross-sectional study was conducted in two phases. In phase 1, after obtaining the demographic information, education level and medical data the participants were distributed into two groups according to whether FOG was present or not. The allocation of the participants to one of two groups, FOG and non-FOG, was done according to question 14 of the UPDRS (functional subscale, part II) which addresses whether the freezing phenomenon was experienced at the time of enrollment. Those who scored one or higher were allocated to the FOG group and those who scored 0 were allocated to the non-FOG group. A neurologist assessed all participants in order to confirm/exclude the presence of FOG in these patients at the time of the test [9]. At the end of this phase, the MEC test was administered. In phase two, only those who obtained a score in MEC ≥ 24 and gave consent to participate in further assessment carried out the remaining assessments previously described.

Statistical analysis


Data analysis was performed using the SPSS Statistics v. 20.0 software. Shapiro-Wilk’s distribution analysis was applied to determine data distribution and the adequate statistical test according to parametric and non-parametric data. To describe profile sample for all variables, data were presented in frequency tables for categorical data and descriptive statistics were applied for numerical data with means and standard deviation. Chi-square and the Fisher test were used to compare categorical data and one-way ANOVA and Mann-Whitney U-test were used for comparisons involving continuous data.

To verify the relationship between cognition and kinematic parameters we conducted a partial correlation controlled by cognitive function, memory, motor condition (UPDRS-III) and academic level. Finally, we performed a multivariate regression analysis to determine a model that allows for identifying the differences between FOG and no-FOG participants. The level of significance for all variables was 5%.
 

Results


Thirty-four PD patients were initially evaluated. Ten participants were classified as freezers (FOG group) and 24 as non-freezers (non-FOG group). Nine participants of non-FOG group screened positive for probable dementia and were thus excluded. Twenty-five individuals completed all evaluations and were included for data analysis. Figure displays the distribution of the sample and excluded cases. 25 individuals completed all evaluations and were included for data analysis. After performing the specific tests, 15 individuals were classified as non-freezers and were included on the non-FOG group and 10 individuals with freezing of gait characteristics were included on FOG group. There were no individuals with advanced PD treatments such as apomorphine, duodopa or deep brain stimulation.

 

Figure. Inclusion of participants for data analysis.






 

Table I shows clinical, cognitive and kinematic characteristics for freezers and non-freezers.
 

Table I. Clinical and kinematic profile and group comparison of outcomes for FOG and non-FOG groups (mean ± standard deviation).
 

Non-FOG (n = 15)

FOG (n = 10)

p a


Clinical parameters

Gender (male/female)

8/7

6/4



Age (years)

69.5 ± 7.99

69.7 ± 5.2

0.93


Academic level (primary/secondary)

10/5

9/1



Disease duration (years)

8.2 ± 4.2

9.0 ± 5.3

0.73


Clinical stage, H&Y

2.6 ± 0.6

2.3 ± 0.5

0.34


Functional, UPDRS II

13.8 ± 4.4

11.8 ± 2.8

0.19


Motor, UPDRS III

12.2 ± 4.1

15.0 ± 2.0

0.02 b


Balance, SPPB

4.0 ± 0.8

3.1 ± 1.3

0.01 b


Cognitive parameters

Global cognition, MEC

31.7 ± 2.3

30.7 ± 2.4

0.02 b


Executive function, FAB

14.3 ± 2.6

13.7 ± 3.0

0.04 b


Memory, FOME

42.3 ± 7.5

40.5 ± 3.7

0.04 b


Kinematic parameters

Cadence (steps/min)

96.0 ± 15.4

111.8 ± 13.5

0.02 b


Gait speed (m/s)

1.9 ± 0.5

1.7 ± 0.5

0.43


Stride length (cm)

62.4 ± 13.8

51.2 ± 10.9

0.04 b


Stride time (s)

1.1 ± 0.1

1.3 ± 0.2

0.01 b


Single support (s)

44.6 ± 6.4

51.0 ± 13.1

0.07


Double support (s)

39.9 ± 9.5

46.4 ± 15.8

0.21


Single/double support time

1.1 ± 0.2

1.1 ± 0.2

0.99


Hip-flexion (degree)

23.9 ± 3.2

23.3 ± 3.8

0.71


Hip-extension (degree)

33.2 ± 2.8

34.2 ± 3.6

0.47


Knee terminal state (degree)

16.2 ± 2.6

15.5 ± 3.9

0.63


Knee-swing phase (degree)

65.4 ± 9.9

67.1 ± 13.1

0.73


Ankle-dorsiflexion (degree)

4.4 ± 2.1

6.8 ± 2.8

0.25


Ankle-plantar flexion (degree)

11.1 ± 3.4

9.5 ± 2.5

0.23


FAB: Frontal Assessment Battery; FOG: freezing of gait; FOME: Fuld Object Memory Evaluation; H&Y: Hoehn & Yahr scale; MEC: miniexamen cognitivo; SPPB: Short Physical Performance Battery; UPDRS: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale. a Unpaired Student’s t. b Statistically significant values.

 

FOG group individuals presented more impaired motor conditions (UPDRS III) (p = 0.02) than non-FOG individuals. Freezers scored significantly worse in global cognition (p = 0.02), executive function (p = 0.04) and memory (p = 0.04). Regarding kinematic parameters, freezers presented significantly increased cadence (p = 0.02), decreased stride length (p = 0.04) and slower stride time (p = 0.01). The combination of small and slow steps with fast cadence resulted in impairments for gait performance on freezers.

Variables with significant differences between both groups were inserted in a Pearson’s correlation and were calculated taking into account the binary outcomes. Regarding this analysis score, UPDRS-III, SPPB, MEC, FOME and FAB, as well as cadence, stride length and stride time, were correlated with FOG showing the greatest values of significant correlation (r > 0.4; p < 0.05).

Values of significant correlation coefficient for each variable are displayed on table II. The results of the univariate analysis performed in order to identify the variables containing significant predictive values are shown on table III.

 

Table II. Relationship between executive function (FAB) and kinematic parameters.
 

FAB

Total
(n = 25)

Non-FOG
(n = 15)

FOG
(n = 10)


Level academic, global cognition, motor (UPDRS III)
and memory (FOME)


Gait speed

r

0.671

0.715

0.809


Sig.

0.001

0.110

0.000


Cadence

r

0.536

0.770

0.303


Sig.

0.003

0.073

0.222


Stride length

r

0.537

0.656

0.795


Sig.

0.003

0.157

0.000


Stride time

r

0.605

0.592

0.566

Sig.

0.001

0.216

0.014


Single support

r

0.583

0.704

0.634


Sig.

0.001

0.118

0.005


Double support

r

–0.583

–0.704

–0.634


Sig.

0.001

0.118

0.005


Single/double support

r

0.610

0.770

0.664


Sig.

0.001

0.073

0.003


Hip-flexion

r

–0.027

0.553

–0.223


Sig.

0.893

0.255

0.373


Hip-extension

r

0.172

–0.150

0.309


Sig.

0.380

0.777

0.213


Knee-terminal state

r

–0.046

–0.138

–0.166


Sig.

0.817

0.794

0.510


Knee-swing phase

r

–0.367

–0.346

–0.338


Sig.

0.055

0.502

0.170


Ankle-dorsiflexion

r

–0.039

0.371

0.338


Sig.

0.844

0.469

0.171


Ankle-plantar flexion

r

–0.158

–0.298

–0.156


Sig.

0.421

0.567

0.537


FAB: Frontal Assessment Battery; FOG: freezing of gait; FOME: Fuld Object Memory Evaluation; H&Y: Hoehn & Yahr scale; MEC: miniexamen cognitivo; r: correlation coefficient Pearson; Sig.: significance; UPDRS: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale.

 

Table III. Relationship adjusted coefficient between FOG and non-FOG groups.
 

R2

p


Motor (UPDRS III)

16.0

0.045


Balance (SPPB)

16.2

0.045


Global cognition (MEC)

19.8

0.026


Executive function (FAB)

16.4

0.044


Memory (FOME)

16.3

0.046


Cadence (steps/min)

21.7

0.029


Stride length (cm)

21.7

0.021


Stride time (s)

17.6

0.035


FAB: Frontal Assessment Battery; FOG: freezing of gait; FOME: Fuld Object Memory Evaluation; H&Y: Hoehn & Yahr scale; MEC: miniexamen cognitivo; R2: adjusted point biserial coefficient; UPDRS: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale.

 

Multivariate analysis was calculated for motor, balance, global cognition, executive function memory, cadence, stride length and stride time. Results are displayed on table IV. These eight contributors, jointly explained approximately 73.9% of variability between FOG and non-FOG patients (R2 = 0.581; p < 0.02).

 

Table IV. Results of multivariate linear regression between FOG and non-FOG patients.
 

Estimate (B)

95% CI


Constant

–6.20

–12.12

–0.28


Motor (UPDRS III)

0.09

0.05

0.14


Balance (SPPB)

0.20

0.04

0.37


Global cognition (MEC)

0.01

–0.09

0.09


Executive function (FAB)

–0.04

–0.10

0.02


Memory (FOME)

0.01

–0.02

0.004


Cadence (steps/min)

0.02

–0.01

0.04


Stride length (cm)

–0.01

–0.03

0.01


Stride time (s)

2.77

1.31

4.23


95% CI: 95% confidence interval; FAB: Frontal Assessment Battery; FOG: freezing of gait; FOME: Fuld Object Memory Evaluation; H&Y: Hoehn & Yahr scale; MEC: miniexamen cognitivo; UPDRS: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale. Only variables with significant predictive values on univariate analysis were inserted to this model. All of these variables showed an independent contribution to explain partially the variability between FOG and non-FOG outcomes.

 

 

Discussion


Our study shows that cognitive and motor scores appear to be strongly correlated with FOG in PD patients. Among the cognitive domains considered, global cognition, memory and executive function contribute to FOG. Of the kinematic parameters, the combination of small and fast steps and increased cadence is the pattern impacting on FOG gait performance. Neither age, gender or disease duration seem to contribute to the FOG phenomenon.

Freezing of gait is an intriguing, complex and ominous motor PD phenomenon. Various models have been suggested as theoretical frameworks in understanding the FOG episodes: a threshold model in which the accumulation of motor deficits over time leads to FOG [24], an interference model proposing that FOG is a result of the inability to deal with central processing resources [25], a cognitive model viewing FOG as induced by a failure to process response conflict [26], and the decoupling model that views FOG as a disconnection between central motor programs and motor response [27].

The finding that performance on cognitive tests differentiates freezers from non-freezers supports the notion that cognitive parameters may have an important contribution to the manifestation of freezing of gait in PD.

In the present work, as previously reported [28], freezers showed worse performance on the MEC than non-freezers, suggesting a significant difference in the global cognitive status between the groups. The fact that global cognition may play a determinant role in FOG is not surprising. In fact, the FOG phenomenon has been related to impairments in dual-task performance (the ability to maintain normal walking while performing a secondary task) and attentional shifting [29]. A specific deficit in monitoring for self-made errors under high cognitive load has been recently reported [30]. Freezers performed also significantly worse than non-freezers on specific cognitive tests such as the FAB and the FOME, revealing more impairment in frontal executive functions and in immediate memory function (more difficulty identifying and recalling familiar household objects by touch or visual processing), respectively. Lower scores on cognitive tests related to frontal lobe and executive functions have been previously reported in freezers [31], suggesting that executive function is a significant predictor of FOG. Importantly, both automatic and controlled (frontal executive function) processes have been shown to be more impaired in freezers than in non-freezers in previous studies [32]. Consistent with these results, there is recent neuroimaging evidence revealing that freezers show relatively reduced functional resting connectivity within both executive-attention and visual neural networks [33] and functional decoupling between the right-lateralized cognitive control (executive) network and the basal ganglia nuclei [34].

Working memory has been shown to be impaired in patients with freezing of gait compared to non-freezers [35]. Immediate memory function, measured by the FOME test, was also significantly lower in freezers than in non-freezers in the present work, suggesting that memory dysfunction may be also an independent determinant of FOG. The role of memory on gait mechanisms, especially in those associated with cadence, has been previously reported [36,37]. When combined with executive dysfunction, memory impairment has been associated with gait speed and predicted longitudinal gait speed decline over five years [38].

The role played by cognitive dysfunction in FOG is also supported by the lack of improvement with levodopa in on-state FOG, suggesting that other neural systems may contribute to its pathogenesis. A study showed that patients with levodopa-unresponsive FOG displayed greater impairments in executive functioning as compared to controls. These findings implicate frontal lobe dysfunction in addition to progression of the pathological process to non-dopaminergic circuits [39]. Several studies have explored the involvement of different neurotransmitters in FOG pathogenesis, specially the noradrenergic and cholinergic systems. Noradrenergic deficits due to neuronal loss in the locus coeruleus have been linked to FOG [40]. Methylphenidate, a drug that inhibits dopamine and noradrenaline presynaptic transporters in the striatum and prefrontal cortex, may improve gait parameters and FOG in patients with advanced PD [41]. In addition to this motor effect, methylphenidate is known to improve attention and executive dysfunction in other disorders [42]. The pedunculo-pontine nucleus, a cholinergic area that is part of the mesencephalic locomotor region, may be implicated in many motor deficits relating to locomotion and posture in PD patients. Central cholinergic potentiation with rivas­tigmine, a central acetilcholesterase inhibitor, and nuclear in antidementia pharmacological therapy may reduce the risk for falls [43].

Our study has several limitations. The size of our sample is small and therefore our results should be interpreted with caution. We did not apply depression and anxiety scales, which recently have been an area of focus in the study in FOG. We also did not adjust our results for treatments, which may interfere with motor and cognitive assessments.

In conclusion, our results suggest that FOG is not a pure motor phenomenon and that it may be associated with global and executive cognitive dysfunction. Because cognitive functions can be significantly improved by cognitive training [44], the present results may have important clinical implications for developing future non-pharmacological intervention and cognitive rehabilitation strategies targeted to improve FOG symptoms in PD patients.

 


Bibliografía
 



 1.    Giladi N, Niewboer A. Understanding and treating freezing of gait in parkinsonism, proposed working definition, and setting the stage. Mov Disord 2008; 23: S423-5.

 2.    Bloem BR, Hausdorff JM, Visser JE, Giladi N. Falls and freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: a review of two interconnected, episodic phenomena. Mov Disord 2004; 19: 871-84.

 3.    Espay AJ, Fasano A, Van Nuenen BF, Payne MM, Snijders AH, Bloem BR. ‘On’ state freezing of gait in Parkinson disease: a paradoxical levodopa-induced complication. Neurology 2012; 78: 454-7.

 4.    Okuma Y. Freezing of gait and falls in Parkinson’s disease. J Parkinsons Dis 2014; 4: 255-60.

 5.    Nutt JG, Bloem BR, Giladi N, Hallett M, Horak FB, Nieuwboer A. Freezing of gait: moving forward on a mysterious clinical phenomenon. Lancet Neurol 2011; 10: 734-44.

 6.    Franco V, Turner RS. Testing the contributions of striatal dopamine loss to the genesis of parkinsonian signs. Neurobiol Dis 2012; 47: 114-25.

 7.    Fytagoridis A, Silburn PA, Coyne TJ, Thevathasan W. Understanding the human pedunculopontine nucleus in Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2016; 123: 769-74.

 8.    Peterson DS, King LA, Cohen RG, Horak FB. Cognitive contributions to freezing of gait in Parkinson disease: implications for physical rehabilitation. Phys Ther 2016; 96: 659-70.

 9.    Ble A, Volpato S, Zuliani G, Guralnik JM, Bandinelli S, Lauretani F, et al. Executive function correlates with walking speed in older persons: the InCHIANTI study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53: 410-5.

 10.  Knobl P, Kielstra L, Almeida Q. The relationship between motor planning and freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2012; 83: 98-101.

 11.  Almeida QJ, Lebold CA. Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: a perceptual cause for a motor impairment? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2010; 81: 513-8.

 12.  Arias P, Cudeiro J. Effects of rhythmic sensory stimulation (auditory, visual) on gait in Parkinson’s disease patients. Exp Brain Res 2008; 186: 589-601.

 13.  Snijders AH, Nijkrake MJ, Bakker M, Munneke M, Wind C, Bloem BR. Clinimetrics of freezing of gait. Mov Disord 2008; 23 (Suppl 2): S468-74.

 14.  Hughes AJ, Ben-Shlomo Y, Daniel SE, Lees AJ. What features improve the accuracy of clinical diagnosis in Parkinson’s disease: a clinicopathologic study. Neurology 1992; 42: 1142-6.

 15.  Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology 1967; 17: 427-42.

 16.  Fahn S, Elton R. Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale. In Fahn S, Lieberman A, eds. Recent developments in Parkinson’s disease. Florham Park, NJ: MacMillan Health Care Information; 1987. p. 153-63.

 17.  Martínez-Martín P, Gil-Nagel A, Gracia LM, Gómez JB, Martínez-Sarriés J, Bermejo F. Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale characteristics and structure. The Cooperative Multicentric Group. Mov Disord 1994; 9: 76-83.

 18.  Guralnik JM, Simonsick EM, Ferruci L, Glynn RJ, Berkman LF, Blazer DG, et al. A short physical performance battery assessing lower extremity function: association with self-reported disability and prediction of mortality and nursing home admission. J Gerontol 1994; 49: M85-94.

 19.  Lobo A, Saz P, Marcos G, Día JL, De la Cámara C, Ventura T, et al. Revalidation and standardization of the cognition mini-exam (first Spanish version of the Mini-Mental Status Examination) in the general geriatric population. Med Clin (Barc) 1999; 112: 767-74.

 20.  Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B. The FAB: a Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology 2000; 55: 1621-6.

 21.  Rodríguez del Álamo A, Catalán-Alonso MJ, Carrasco-Marín L. FAB: aplicación preliminar española de la batería neuro-psicológica de evaluación de funciones frontales a 11 grupos de pacientes. Rev Neurol 2003; 36: 605-8.

 22.  La Rue A, Romero LJ, Ortiz IE, Liang HC, Lindeman RD. Neuropsychological performance of Hispanic and non-Hispanic older adults: an epidemiologic survey. Clin Neuropsychol 1999; 13: 474-86.

 23.  Fuld PA, Masur DM, Blau AD, Crystal H, Aronson MK. Object-memory evaluation for prospective detection of dementia in normal functioning elderly: predictive and normative data. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1990; 12: 520-8.

 24. Plotnik M, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. Is freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease a result of multiple gait impairments? Implications for treatment. Parkinsons Dis 2012; 2012: 459321.

 25.  DeLong MR, Wichmann T. Circuits and circuit disorders of the basal ganglia. Arch Neurol 2007; 64: 20-4.

 26.  D’Ostilio, K, Garraux G. Brain mechanisms underlying automatic and unconscious control of motor action. Front Hum Neurosci 2012; 6: 225.

 27.  Rocchi L, Carlson-Kuhta P, Chiari L, Burchiel KJ, Hogarth P, Horak FB. Effects of deep brain stimulation in the subthalamic nucleus or globus pallidus internus on step initiation in Parkinson disease: laboratory investigation. J Neurosurg 2012; 117: 1141-9.

 28.  Heremans E, Nieuwboer A, Spildooren J, Vandenbossche J, Deroost N, Soetens E, et al. Cognitive aspects of freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: a challenge for rehabilitation. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2013; 120: 543-57.

 29.  Amboni M, Cozzolino A, Longo K, Picillo M, Barone P. Freezing of gait and executive functions in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2008; 23: 395-400.

 30.  Walton CC, Shine JM, Mowszowski L, Gilat M, Hall JM, O’Callaghan C, et al. Impaired cognitive control in Parkinson’s disease patients with freezing of gait in response to cognitive load. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2015; 122: 653-60.

 31.  Dirnberger G, Jahanshahi M. Executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: a review. J Neuropsychol 2013; 7: 193-224.

 32.  Vandenbossche J, Deroost N, Soetens E, Coomans D, Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, et al. Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: disturbances in automaticity and control. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 6: 356.

 33.  Tessitore A, Amboni M, Esposito F, Russo A, Picillo M, Marcuccio L, et al. Resting-state brain connectivity in patients with Parkinson’s disease and freezing of gait. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2012; 18: 781-7.

 34.  Shine JM, Matar E, Ward PB, Frank MJ, Moustafa AA, Pearson M, et al. Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease is associated with functional decoupling between the cognitive control network and the basal ganglia. Brain 2013; 136: 3671-81.

 35.  Hall JM, Shine JM, Walton CC, Gilat M, Kamsma YP, Naismith SL et al. Early phenotypic differences between Parkinson’s disease patients with and without freezing of gait. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2014; 20: 604-7.

 36.  Amboni M, Barone P, Hausdorff JM. Cognitive contributions to gait and falls: evidence and implications. Mov Disord 2013; 28: 1520-33.

 37.  Holtzer R, Wang C, Verghese J. The relationship between attention and gait in aging: facts and fallacies. Motor Control 2012; 16: 64-80.

 38.  Watson NL, Rosano C, Boudreau RM, Simonsick EM, Ferrucci L, Sutton-Tyrrell K, et al. Executive function, memory, and gait speed decline in well-functioning older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2010; 65: 1093-100.

 39.  Ferraye MU, Ardouin C, Lhommée E, Fraix V, Krack P, Chabardès S, et al. Levodopa-resistant freezing of gait and executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Eur Neurol 2013; 69: 281-8.

 40.  Tohgi H, Abe T, Takahashi S, Nozaki Y, Ueno M, Kikuchi T. Monoamine metabolism in the cerebrospinal fluid in Parkinson’s disease: relationship to clinical symptoms and subsequent therapeutic outcomes. J Neural Transm Park Dis Dement Sect 1993; 5: 17-26.

 41.  Moreau C, Delval A, Defebvre L, Dujardin K, Duhamel A, Petyt G, et al; for the Parkgait-II Study Group. Methylphenidate for gait hypokinesia and freezing in Parkinson patients undergoing subthalamic stimulation: a multicentre, parallel, placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Neurol 2012; 11: 589-96.

 42.  Tamminga HG, Reneman L, Huizenga HM, Geurts HM. Effects of methylphenidate on executive functioning in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan: a meta-regression analysis. Psychol Med 2016; 46: 1791-807.

 43.  Henderson EH, Lord SR, Brodie MA, Gaunt DM, Lawrence AD, Close JCT, et al. Rivastigmine for gait stability in patients with Parkinson’s disease (ReSPonD): a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, phase 2 trial. Lancet Neurol 2016; 15: 249-58.

 44.  Millán-Calenti JC, Lorenzo T, Núñez-Naveira L, Buján A, Rodríguez-Villamil JL, Maseda A. Efficacy of a computerized cognitive training application on cognition and depressive symptomatology in a group of healthy older adults: a randomized controlled trial. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2015; 61: 337-43.

 

Influencia del deterioro cognitivo en la congelación de la marcha en pacientes con enfermedad de Parkinson sin demencia

Introducción. La congelación de la marcha (CDM) es una alteración motora que suele aparecer en estadios avanzados de la enfermedad de Parkinson (EP). Las funciones cognitivas y ejecutivas parecen tener un papel importante en la aparición de este fenómeno.

Objetivo. Investigar si los parámetros cognitivos y cinemáticos se correlacionan con la CDM en pacientes con EP sin demencia.

Pacientes y métodos. Estudio observacional y transversal. Los participantes se clasificaron en dos grupos: con y sin CDM. La información clínica se obtuvo mediante la escala de Hoehn y Yahr, la Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale y la prueba de equilibrio de la Short Physical Performance Battery. La función cognitiva se valoró con el miniexamen cognitivo y la Fuld Object Memory Evaluation, y la función ejecutiva, con la Frontal Assessment Battery. Los parámetros cinemáticos se valoraron mediante la velocidad de la marcha, la cadencia, la longitud del paso y el tiempo del paso.

Resultados. Veinticinco participantes con EP sin demencia completaron el programa. Se encontraron diferencias estadísticamente significativas entre individuos con y sin CDM en cognición global (p = 0,02), memoria (p = 0,04), función ejecutiva (p = 0,04), cadencia (p = 0,02), longitud del paso (p = 0,04) y tiempo del paso (p = 0,01).
Conclusión. Diversos parámetros cognitivos pueden contribuir de forma importante en la aparición de la CDM en la EP.
Estos resultados pueden tener implicaciones clínicas relevantes para el desarrollo de estrategias e intervenciones no farmacológicas y cognitivas dirigidas a pacientes con EP y con CDM.

Palabras clave. Cognición. Congelación de la marcha. Enfermedad de Parkinson. Marcha. Personas mayores. Trastorno motor.

 

© 2018 Revista de Neurología

Si ya es un usuario registrado en Neurologia, introduzca sus datos de inicio de sesión.


Rellene los campos para registrarse en Neurologia.com y acceder a todos nuestros artículos de forma gratuita
Datos básicos
He leído y acepto la política de privacidad y el aviso legal
Seleccione la casilla si desea recibir el número quincenal de Revista de Neurología por correo electrónico. De forma quincenal se le mandará un correo con los títulos de los artículos publicados en Revista de Neurología.
Seleccione la casilla si desea recibir el boletín semanal de Revista de Neurología por correo electrónico. El boletín semanal es una selección de las noticias publicadas diariamente en Revista de Neurología.
Seleccione la casilla si desea recibir información general de neurologia.com (Entrevistas, nuevos cursos de formación, eventos, etc.)
Datos complementarios

Se os solicita los datos de redes para dar repercusión por estos medios a las publicaciones en las que usted participe.



Estimado usuario de Revista de Neurología,

Debido a la reciente fusión por absorción de VIGUERA EDITORES, S.L.U., la entidad gestora de las publicaciones de Viguera Editores, entre ellas, Revista de Neurología, por EVIDENZE HEALTH ESPAÑA, S.L.U., una de las sociedades también pertenecientes al Grupo Evidenze, y con la finalidad de que Usted pueda seguir disfrutando de los contenidos y distintos boletines a los que está suscrito en la página web de neurologia.com, es imprescindible que revise la nueva política de privacidad y nos confirme la autorización de la cesión de sus datos.

Lamentamos informarle que en caso de no disponer de su consentimiento, a partir del día 28 de octubre no podrá acceder a la web de neurologia.com

Para dar su consentimiento a seguir recibiendo la revista y los boletines de neurologia.com vía correo electrónico y confirmar la aceptación de la nueva política de privacidad, así como la cesión de sus datos a Evidenze Health España S.L.U., el resto de las entidades del Grupo Evidenze y sus partners y colaboradores comerciales, incluyendo la posibilidad de llevar a cabo transferencias internacionales a colaboradores extranjeros, pulse en el siguiente enlace:

ACEPTAR

Cancelar

Atentamente

El equipo de Revista de Neurología