Original

How does methylphenidate affect default mode network? A systematic review

P.H. Santos, R. Gonçalves, S. Pedroso [REV NEUROL 2019;68:417-425] PMID: 31070233 DOI: https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.6810.2018487 OPEN ACCESS
Volumen 68 | Number 10 | Nº of views of the article 142.178 | Nº of PDF downloads 701 | Article publication date 16/05/2019
Icono-PDF-OFF Download PDF Castellano Icono-PDF-OFF Download PDF English Citation Search in PubMed
Share in: Facebook Twitter
Go to another issue
ABSTRACT Artículo en español English version
INTRODUCTION Methylphenidate is a widely-used drug for the treatment of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and other neuropsychiatric disorders. Sustained-attention deficits and poorer task performance in these disorders have been associated with default mode network (DMN) dysfunction in fMRI studies. DMN is a set of brain areas more activated during the resting-state. Under the execution of external tasks, there is an attenuation of DMN activity. In healthy individuals, DMN and task-positive network are anticorrelated. It has been suggested that methylphenidate could normalize the attenuated task-related DMN deactivation in attention- and inhibitory control-related disorders and that such normalization could improve task performance.

PATIENTS AND METHODS To explore the hypothesis of DMN deactivation after methylphenidate administration, we conducted a systematic review of the literature.

RESULTS After a systematic search, 12 studies were included in this review. For eligibility, studies were required to measure the effects of methylphenidate administration on the DMN activity. Eleven studies showed evidence of MPH-induced improvements in brain areas related to DMN. The results suggest a normalization of brain circuits in individuals with DMN dysfunction.

CONCLUSIONS Our preliminary findings strongly suggest methylphenidate improves DMN dysfunction presented in ADHD and other neuropsychiatric disorders. Further studies are needed to better understand this effect and expand comprehension of methylphenidate action mechanisms.
KeywordsADHDAttention deficit/hyperactivity disorderDefault mode networkDMNMethylphenidateNeurosciencePsychopharmacologyResting-state CategoriesNeurociencia básicaNeuropsiquiatría
FULL TEXT Artículo en español English version

Introduction


Methylphenidate mechanism of action and evidence for treatment


Methylphenidate (MPH) is a widely-used drug for the treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and other neuropsychiatric disorders, in order to enhance attention and cognition, including reducing reaction time, enhancing perceptual sensitivity, working memory, speed of processing, verbal learning, attention and vigilance [1-4]. MPH blocks dopamine and norepinephrine transporters, thereby decreasing presynaptic reuptake following release and increasing extracellular concentration of these neuromodulatory neurotransmitters in the synaptic cleft [5-7]. Basal ganglia, mainly striatum, frontal lobes and cerebellum have been found to be MPH targeted areas in the brain [8,9].

MPH is recommended as first-line pharmacological ADHD treatment by updated guidelines [10]. While the exact pathophysiology of ADHD is not completely understood, treatment with stimulants has long been recognized to attenuate symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity in children with the disorder [11,12]. Furthermore, research on MPH also revealed potential for the treatment of cocaine addiction and narcolepsy and for the recovery from traumatic brain injury (TBI) [13-15]. The prescribing rates of MPH increased dramatically in the past two decades [16]. Awareness to adverse drug reactions must be taken, however, data concerning safety issues shows that, despite being common, adverse drug reactions are mild in severity, both for pediatric and adult populations [17-19].

Default mode network structures and dysfunction-related disorders


Default mode network (DMN) is part of accidental science [20]. Despite being initially neglected [21], DMN functions were progressively hypothesized, both in emotional and cognitive processes integration and future planning through episodic memory retrieval [22,23]. Regions reported to compound DMN are the ventrolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, the posterior cingulate cortex, the cuneus, and the inferior parietal lobe [24]. DMN is a brain network more activated at rest, when there is no focus on specific tasks, which deactivates when an external task is performed and the magnitude of deactivation is associated to the task demand and engagement, memory load and stimulus rate [24-28]. DMN and task-positive network (TPN) are anticorrelated during an externally oriented task [29-32]. Coordination between DMN and TPN seems to be necessary for efficient inhibitory control [33].

DMN dysfunction has been related to abnormal mental processes and illnesses, including ADHD and TBI [34-36]. Among the main abnormal neuropsychological processes that have been related to DMN dysfunction are attention and inhibitory control deficits. Sustained-attention deficits of ADHD were early associated with altered DMN coherence, particularly intrusive DMN activity [29]. Poorer task performance was associated with the failure to attenuate DMN activity during tasks, particularly if they were attentionally demanding [37-39]. Greater response time variability was reported when DMN and TPN anticorrelation was weaker [30].

Default mode network dysfunction and methylphenidate treatment


MPH has clearly shown its efficacy for ADHD treatment and is recommended as first-line medication on ADHD treatment guidelines [10]. Improvement of symptoms in narcolepsy and TBI has also been reported [14,15]. Dysfunction of DMN activity was also found in ADHD, narcolepsy and TBI [31,36,40].

A few studies reported MPH interactions with DMN activity. Peterson et al demonstrated that MPH normalized the attenuated task-related DMN deactivation in ADHD patients [41]. Additionally, Volkow et al found that decreased brain’s glucose demands during a cognitive task in healthy subjects under MPH reflected enhanced DMN regions deactivation [42].

To explore the hypothesis of DMN deactivation after MPH administration, we conducted a systematic-review of the literature. The results may shed light on DMN functions, attention- and inhibitory control-related disorders and expand comprehension of MPH mechanisms of action. To our knowledge, there are no reviews on the subject.
 

Patients and methods


Search strategy


A systematic search was conducted in PubMed and Cochrane databases up to and including October 2018. The following terms were used to search in both title and text: ‘methylphenidate’ combined with ‘default mode network’. The search was limited to original research studies published in English in peer-reviewed journals. The titles and abstracts of the search results were then screened and the relevant papers identified.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria


Studies were included if they:
 
  • They were human clinical trials that included randomized controlled trials, non-randomized trials, case studies and/or case series.
  • They evaluated a direct effect of MPH administration on the DMN functioning.
  • They were written in English.

Studies were excluded if:
 
  • They did not present new or unique data (review articles, letters to the editor, duplicate articles).
  • They did not present an outcome measure related to the effect on DMN.
  • They did not use MPH to evaluate the outcomes.

In the first stage of the selection process, all papers whose title or abstract did not meet the inclusion criteria were excluded. From the initial 28 identified papers, we excluded 14. In the second stage of the selection process, the full text of remaining papers was read to exclude those not relevant to the review. At the end, two papers were excluded because they did not present a measure of DMN activity after MPH administration. The final review included 12 papers.

This systematic review was reported using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines. The search process is summarized in the figure.

 

Figure. Flowchart of published studies examined for inclusion in the systematic review.






 


Results


Characteristics of included studies


Twelve studies, involving 480 participants, were included, addressing the effects of MPH in DMN areas. The demographic and clinical features of the samples were very heterogeneous and three studies did not refer the mean age of the samples. In the remaining studies, the mean age varied between 8.78 ± 0.85 and 39.9 ± 5.5 years old, with five studies using only pediatric samples. Five studies used only male subjects in their sample and all of the studies had a disproportionally predominance of males. The studies varied also in the clinical features of the sample. While the majority of studies tested the administration of MPH in ADHD patients (five MPH naïve, two MPH responsive patients and one previously MPH-treated patients washed-out), two studies administered MPH in healthy subjects, one used a sample of cocaine dependent patients and another tested MPH in patients seven months after TBI with diffuse axonal injury.

There was also a great variability in the methodologies and the brain regions analyzed across the different studies. Nine studies were controlled, while three lacked a control group. Five studies administered placebo in the experimental or in the control group. Oral administration of MPH was the most frequently used form of administration, with only one study preferring intravenous administration. Three of them tried to analyze the long-term effects of treatment with MPH, performing an evaluation after a period of more than one month of treatment.

Among the studies, ten evaluated DMN activity through fMRI scan, one used EEG and another used steady-state visual evoked potential. The majority of them evaluated the effects of MPH in DMN functioning during the performance of a neuropsychological task (requiring visual attention, inhibitory control or working memory), while two evaluated the effects of MPH in DMN functioning during resting-state only.

The complete characterization of the studies included is presented in the table.

 

Table. Summary of studies discussed in the text.
 
Subjects

Intervention

Main findings on DMN activity after MPH administration

Patients (MPH)

Controls

Tomasi
et al [43]


16 healthy males (mean age: 33 ± 3 years)

16 healthy males (mean age: 36 ± 2 years)

Patients: oral administration of 20 mg MPH

Controls: no pharmacological intervention

Deactivation

Liddle
et al [44]


18 ADHD-C subtype MPH responsive (9-15 years)

18 typically developed (9-15 years)

Patients: regular oral MPH and 36 h after off-MPH, under different motivational conditions

Controls: no pharmacological intervention

Deactivation

Marquand
et al [45]


15 healthy (20-39 years)



Patients: oral administration of 30 mg
MPH and placebo

Deactivation

Matuskey
et al [53]


10 cocaine dependent (DSM-IV-R criteria) (8 male; mean age: 39.9 ± 5.5 years)



Patients: intravenous administration of MPH (0.5 mg/kg) and placebo

Increased activation: normalization of neural processes

Querne
et al [46]


11 ADHD MPH-naïve (8-13 years; mean age: 9.8 ± 1.7 years)

11 typically developed (8-13 years; mean age: 10.8 ± 1.7 years)

Patients: performed twice, once before MPH and once after a month of continuous MPH (20 or 30 mg, extended release formulation)

Controls: no pharmacological intervention

Deactivation

Cooper
et al [47]


38 ADHD non-treated males and 17 on follow-up (on MPH) (18-58 years; mean age: 28.5 ± 9.5 years)

43 no-ADHD males and 34 on follow-up (19-65 years; mean age: 29.0 ± 10.4 years)

Patients: tested before the beginning of treatment with MPH and after a follow-up with MPH (mean follow-up: 9.4 months)

Controls: no pharmacological intervention

Deactivation

Salavert
et al [48]


41 ADHD patients: 26 under chronic MPH treatment and 15 treatment naive (28 males, 13 females)

41 healthy controls

Patients: Discontinued from medication at least four days prior to the fMRI

Controls: no pharmacological intervention

Deactivation

Battel
et al [54]


23 MPH-naïve ADHD boys (8-10 years; mean age: 8.78 ± 0.85 years); 21 at 6-months follow-up)



Patients: individual titrated MPH treatment (target dosage of 1 mg/kg/day)

No significant difference

Silk et al [49]

16 ADHD male: 10 MPH-naïve, 6 under 48 h MPH wash out (9-18 years; mean age: 13.4 ± 2.4 years)

15 typically developed male (mean age: 14.4 ± 2.5 years)

Patients: 20 mg MPH single dose administration and placebo (two weeks or more of interval)

Controls: placebo

Deactivation

Moreno-López et al [50]

14 with > 7 months TBI patients with diffuse axonal injury: 10 male, 4 female (mean age: 36.86 ± 14.17 years)

20 healthy: 12 male, 8 female (mean age: 34.15 ± 11.12 years)

Patients: 30 mg oral MPH and placebo

Controls: no pharmacological intervention

Deactivation

Silberstein
et al [51]


42 ADHD treatment naïve boys (mean age: 10.04 years ± 2.00 years)

25 healthy boys (mean age: 10.83 ± 1.74 years)

Patients: 0.3 mg/kg of MPH

Controls: no pharmacological intervention

Deactivation

Mowinckel
et al [52]


20 ADHD adults under MPH treatment: 7 male, 13 female (mean age: 29.90 ± 1.41 years)

27 healthy adults: 8 male, 19 female (mean age: 27.42 ± 1.23 years)

Patients: MPH and placebo administration in randomized order

Controls: no pharmacological intervention

Deactivation

ADHD: attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder; DMN: default mode network; fMRI: functional magnetic resonance imaging; MPH: methylphenidate.

 

Effects of methylphenidate on DMN activity


Eleven studies revealed an effect of MPH administration in the activity of brain areas related to DMN. Among those, ten revealed an increased deactivation of specific DMN areas after MPH administration [43-52]. The study which the sample included cocaine dependent patients showed that MPH increased the activation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and precuneus, before task errors, which translates a normalization of neural processes [53]. One study failed to show any DMN activity differences before and after MPH administration [54].

In the ten studies in which MPH produced DMN deactivation, different areas within DMN were observed to be affected: four studies, posterior cingulate cortex; two, prefrontal cortex; one, medial frontal cortex; one, insula; three, precuneus; one, ventral anterior cingulate cortex; one, temporal gyri; one, angular gyri; and one, frontoparietal regions and other areas involving occipital, temporal and cerebellar regions.
 

Discussion


In this systematic review, we identified 12 studies aiming to assess the effects of MPH administration on DMN functioning. Regarding subjects’ disorders, eight studies included ADHD patients, one included patients with cocaine dependency, one included patients with TBI diagnosis and two studies included only healthy individuals. The diagnoses comprised in the studies, as well as its distribution, were not a surprise, since ADHD is the main targeted disorder for MPH administration and it is among the most well studied DMN dysfunction-related disorders. The hypothesis of intrusive DMN activity on ADHD was supported either by reported disrupted functional connectivity between DMN and other brain regions and reduced DMN homogeneity, both in ADHD child and adult patients [31,55,56]. A meta-analysis pointed DMN hyperactivity among the main findings in ADHD groups compared to controls [57]. In youths with ADHD, the DMN task-related deactivation was found to be attenuated during an inhibitory control task [41]. For TBI populations, abnormally increased DMN connectivity and reduced connectivity between salience and DMN during the execution of response inhibition tasks were reported [58-60]. DMN dysfunction was also associated with slower response inhibition in TBI patients [61].

DMN was found to be a brain network in which its activity is higher at the resting-state and that deactivates when an external task is performed [24]. Those individuals presenting a failure on attenuating DMN activity, during external tasks, had weaker performance on those tasks [37]. For example, the decreased deactivation of DMN was associated with errors in an inhibitory control task [62]. Moreover, intra-individual response time variability during a task was also associated with DMN activation [63]. In this review, nine out of the ten studies that included non-healthy individuals showed restoration of DMN activity after MPH administration. From those, the restoration was reflected by DMN deactivation in eight studies. Interestingly, the same pattern of functional activity was achieved in those two studies including healthy individuals only. When specific deactivated DMN areas were described, posterior cingulate cortex was the most often deactivated region. Indeed, posterior cingulate cortex integrates the core DMN, a subset that deactivates independently of the task [64]. prefrontal cortex, medial frontal cortex, precuneus, ventral anterior cingulate cortex, temporal gyri and angular gyri also showed MPH induced deactivation. Querne et al conducted an evaluation on DMN activity before and one month after initiating MPH treatment, while the subjects were performing a flanker task [46]. Their results showed a progression from no anticorrelation between DMN and TPN activities to an anticorrelation between those networks, one month after MPH treatment, similar to what was observed in the control group. In fact, TPN regions are activated under purposeful attention [65,66]. Since DMN and TPN anticorrelation is needed for efficient external tasks performance, induced DMN deactivation through MPH administration seems to normalize TPN functioning.

The study that included cocaine dependent patients, despite showing increased DMN activation with MPH, reflected normalization of neural processes with restoration of DMN normal activity [53]. The authors found an increased DMN activity preceding stop errors of a stop-signal task, under MPH. This is accordingly to the literature, as performance errors prediction by DMN activation, in healthy individuals, was suggested from the increased activity in key DMN structures during stop signal errors, as compared to the activity during stop signal successes [62]. Also, intravenous administration of MPH improved inhibitory control in non-treatment seeking cocaine using individuals [67]. Taken together, these findings provide support for the efficacy of psychostimulants in remediating self-control in cocaine addicts [68-70].

Only one study failed to show a MPH-induced normalization of DMN activity [54]. However, this was the only study that neither included a control group, placebo administration to the patients group or the performance of a task.

Silk et al, despite conducting the study in a resting-state condition, found reduced DMN activity under MPH, compared to the placebo condition [49]. Previous data demonstrated MPH evident alterations in the resting-state, and such effects were presumably related to a differentiation of signal from noise and restoration of attention focus [71-73]. Two studies included healthy subjects only and their results matched the expected MPH effects on DMN activity [43,45]. Actually, MPH previously demonstrated evidence in response inhibition paradigms both in psychiatric and healthy populations [74,75].

Limitations


This review should be considered in the light of some limitations that should be taken into account when interpreting the results. Firstly, the review is constrained by limitations at study level, due to the quality and methodological differences of the included studies. Scientific literature is still lacking significant studies and data on the specific effects of MPH in DMN and other brain regions. In what concerns to the type of studies included, initially we searched specifically for randomized controlled trials. However, because of the scarcity of randomized controlled trials, we had to reconsider our inclusion criteria and include different study designs, some of them lacking control groups, placebo testing and double-blinded measurements.

Also, the analysis of available data is difficult due to heterogeneity of studied populations. While the majority of studies selected exclusively patients with ADHD, comparing them with typically developing samples, some evaluated the effect of MPH in heterogeneous populations including cocaine dependent individuals or patients after TBI. Even between those that used samples of ADHD patients, the samples were significantly heterogeneous. Some studies started the administration of MPH in MPH-naïve patients, while others preferred patients with ADHD already treated and that showed to be responsive to MPH, and some also mixed these two groups in their samples. The heterogeneity of samples is also relevant with regard to age, showing a markedly broad age range across the studies.

Furthermore, the majority of studies used a predominance of male samples, which lead to caution when generalizing these results to the female population.

Comparison across studies was further hindered by heterogeneity in the methodologies used for the analysis of effects of MPH in the DMN activity, the instruments and measuring techniques, units and statistical procedures employed. The majority of studies tested the effect of MPH in DMN during the performance of a neurocognitive task. However, some studies tested the MPH effect during resting-state, which could minimize their results, because they did not allow observing the deactivation of DMN during a task requiring external attention.

Finally, regarding limitations at review-level, by including only publications written in English, we may have inadvertently excluded relevant publications, even though the literature search was comprehensive and systematic.
 

Conclusions


In this systematic review, we found favorable evidence for the effect of MPH in different areas related to DMN, in multiple clinical populations, which supports the previous literature in the field. Mostly, MPH was able to normalize the DMN and TPN anticorrelation, which is needed for efficient external tasks performance.

Data is still limited in terms of quality and quantity of studies. Further well designed, larger controlled trials, testing against placebos and analyzing the effects of MPH on DMN during the performance of a homogenous attention-task, are needed to offer evidence of sufficient quality for supporting these results and a posterior meta-analysis.

 

References
 


 1.  Ackerman PT, Dykman RA, Holcomb PJ, McCray DS. Methylphenidate effects of cognitive style and reaction time in four groups of children. Psychiatry Res 1982; 7: 199-213.

 2.  Camp-Bruno JA, Herting RL. Cognitive effects of milacemide and methylphenidate in healthy young adults. Psychopharma-cology (Berl) 1994; 115: 46-52.

 3.  Izquierdo I, Bevilaqua LR, Rossato JI, Lima RH, Medina JH, Cammarota M. Age-dependent and age-independent human memory persistence is enhanced by delayed posttraining methylphenidate administration. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2008; 105: 19504-7.

 4.  Linssen AM, Sambeth A, Vuurman EF, Riedel WJ. Cognitive effects of methylphenidate in healthy volunteers: a review of single dose studies. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2014; 17: 961-77.

 5.  Segal DS, Kuczenski R. Effects of methylphenidate on extracellular dopamine, serotonin, and norepinephrine: comparison with amphetamine. J Neurochem 1997; 68: 2032-7.

 6.  Volkow ND, Wang G, Fowler JS, Logan J, Gerasimov M, Maynard L, et al. Therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate significantly increase extracellular dopamine in the human brain. J Neurosci 2001; 21: RC121.

 7.  Gatley SJ, Ding YS, Volkow ND, Chen R, Sugano Y, Fowler JS. Binding of d-threo-[11C]methylphenidate to the dopamine transporter in vivo: insensitivity to synaptic dopamine. Eur J Pharmacol 1995; 281: 141-9.

 8.  Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Ding YS. Imaging the effects of methylphenidate on brain dopamine: new model on its therapeutic actions for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57: 1410-5.

 9.  Czerniak SM, Sikoglu EM, King JA, Kennedy DN, Mick E, Frazier J, et al. Areas of the brain modulated by single-dose methylphenidate treatment in youth with ADHD during task-based fMRI: a systematic review. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2013; 21: 151-62.

 10.  Bolea-Alamanac B, Nutt DJ, Adamou M, Asherson P, Bazire S, Coghill D, et al. Evidence-based guidelines for the pharmacological management of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: update on recommendations from the British Association for Psycho-pharmacology. J Psychopharmacol 2014; 28: 179-203.

 11.  Taylor E, Dopfner M, Sergeant J, Asherson P, Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, et al. European clinical guidelines for hyperkinetic disorder –first upgrade. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2004; 13 (Suppl 1): 7-30.

 12.  Pliszka SR, Liotti M, Bailey BY, Perez R III, Glahn D, Semrud-Clikeman M. Electrophysiological effects of stimulant treatment on inhibitory control in children with attention-deficit/hyper-activity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 2007; 17: 356-66.

 13.  Dursteler KM, Berger EM, Strasser J, Caflisch C, Mutschler J, Herdener M, et al. Clinical potential of methylphenidate in the treatment of cocaine addiction: a review of the current evidence. Subst Abuse Rehabil 2015; 6: 61-74.

 14.  Johansson B, Wentzel AP, Andréll P, Mannheimer C, Rönnbäck L. Methylphenidate reduces mental fatigue and improves processing speed in persons suffered a traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 2015; 29: 758-65.

 15.  Lecendreux M, Lavault S, Lopez R, Inocente CO, Konofal E, Cortese S, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms in pediatric narcolepsy: a cross-sectional study. Sleep 2015; 38: 1285-95.

 16.  Renoux C, Shin JY, Dell’Aniello S, Fergusson E, Suissa S. Prescribing trends of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) medications in UK primary care, 1995-2015. Br J Clin Pharmacol 2016; 82: 858-68.

 17.  Rappley M. Clinical practice. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. N Engl J Med 2005; 352: 165-73.

 18.  Edvinsson D, Ekselius L. Long-term tolerability and safety of pharmacological treatment of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a 6-year prospective naturalistic study. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2018; 38: 370-5.

 19.  Khajehpiri Z, Mahmoudi-Gharaei J, Faghihi T, Karimzadeh I, Khalili H, Mohammadi M. Adverse reactions of methylphenidate in children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: report from a referral center. J Res Pharm Pract 2014; 3: 130-6.

 20.  Buckner RL. The serendipitous discovery of the brain’s default network. Neuroimage 2012; 62: 1137-45.

 21.  Buckner RL, Raichle ME, Miezin FM, Petersen SE. Functional anatomic studies of memory retrieval for auditory words and visual pictures. J Neurosci 1996; 16: 6219-35.

 22.  Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001; 98: 676-82.

 23.  Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2003; 100: 253-8.

 24.  Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008; 1124: 1-38.

 25.  Raichle ME, Snyder AZ. A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage 2007; 37: 1083-90.

 26.  Greicius MD, Menon V. Default-mode activity during a passive sensory task: uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation. J Cogn Neurosci 2004; 16: 1484-92.

 27.  McKiernan KA, D’Angelo BR, Kaufman JN, Binder JR. Interrupting the ‘stream of consciousness’: an fMRI investigation. Neuroimage 2006; 29: 1185-91.

 28.  Singh KD, Fawcett IP. Transient and linearly graded deactivation of the human default-mode network by a visual detection task. Neuroimage 2008; 41: 100-12.

 29.  Sonuga-Barke EJS, Castellanos FX. Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: a neurobiological hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2007; 31: 977-86.

 30.  Clare Kelly AM, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability. Neuroimage 2008; 39: 527-37.

 31.  Castellanos FX, Margulies DS, Kelly C, Uddin LQ, Ghaffari M, Kirsch A, et al. Cingulate-precuneus interactions: a new locus of dysfunction in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 63: 332-7.

 32.  Gerlach KD, Spreng RN, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL. Solving future problems: default network and executive activity associated with goal-directed mental simulations. Neuroimage 2011; 55: 1816-24.

 33.  Sharp DJ, Scott G, Leech R. Network dysfunction after traumatic brain injury. Nat Rev Neurol 2014; 10: 156-66.

 34.  Anticevic A, Cole MW, Murray JD, Corlett PR, Wang XJ, Krystal JH. The role of default network deactivation in cognition and disease. Trends Cogn Sci 2012; 16: 584-92.

 35.  Fair DA, Posner J, Nagel BJ, Bathula D, Dias TGC, Mills KL, et al. Atypical default network connectivity in youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 68: 1084-91.

 36.  Zhou Y, Milham MP, Lui YW, Miles L, Reaume J, Sodickson DK, et al. Default-mode network disruption in mild traumatic brain injury. Radiology 2012; 265: 882-92.

 37.  Weissman DH, Roberts KC, Visscher KM, Woldorff MG. The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat Neurosci 2006; 9: 971-8.

 38.  Eichele T, Debener S, Calhoun VD, Specht K, Engel AK, Hugdahl K, et al. Prediction of human errors by maladaptive changes in event-related brain networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2008; 105: 6173-8.

 39.  Dwyer DB, Harrison BJ, Yucel M, Whittle S, Zalesky A, Pantelis C, et al. Large-scale brain network dynamics supporting adolescent cognitive control. J Neurosci 2014; 34: 14096-107.

 40.  Drissi NM, Szakacs A, Witt ST, Wretman A, Ulander M, Stahlbrandt H, et al. Altered brain microstate dynamics in adolescents with narcolepsy. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10: 369.

 41.  Peterson BS, Potenza MN, Wang Z, Zhu H, Martin A, Marsh R, et al. An fMRI study of the effects of psychostimulants on default-mode processing during Stroop task performance in youths with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166: 1286-94.

 42.  Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Telang F, Logan J, Wong C, et al. Methylphenidate decreased the amount of glucose needed by the brain to perform a cognitive task. PLoS One 2008; 3: e2017.

 43.  Tomasi D, Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Wang R, Telang F, Caparelli EC, et al. Methylphenidate enhances brain activation and deactivation responses to visual attention and working memory tasks in healthy controls. Neuroimage 2011; 54: 3101-10.

 44.  Liddle EB, Hollis C, Batty MJ, Groom MJ, Totman JJ, Liotti M, et al. Task-related default mode network modulation and inhibitory control in ADHD: effects of motivation and methyl-
phenidate. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip 2011; 52: 761-71.

 45.  Marquand AF, De Simoni S, O’Daly OG, Williams SC, Mourao-Miranda J, Mehta MA. Pattern classification of working memory networks reveals differential effects of methylphenidate, atomoxetine, and placebo in healthy volunteers. Neuropsycho-pharmacology 2011; 36: 1237-47.

 46.  Querne L, Fall S, Le Moing AG, Bourel-Ponchel E, Delignières A, Simonnot A, et al. Effects of methylphenidate on default-mode network/task-positive network synchronization in children with ADHD. J Atten Disord 2014; 21: 1208-20.

 47.  Cooper RE, Skirrow C, Tye C, McLoughlin G, Rijsdijk F, Banaschweski T, et al. The effect of methylphenidate on very low frequency electroencephalography oscillations in adult ADHD. Brain Cogn 2014; 86: 82-9.

 48.  Salavert J, Ramos-Quiroga JA, Moreno-Alcázar A, Caseras X, Palomar G, Radua J, et al. Functional imaging changes in the medial prefrontal cortex in adult ADHD. J Atten Disord 2018; 22: 679-93.

 49.  Silk TJ, Malpas C, Vance A, Bellgrove MA. The effect of single-dose methylphenidate on resting-state network functional connectivity in ADHD. Brain Imaging Behav 2017; 11: 1422-31.

 50.  Moreno-López L, Manktelow AE, Sahakian BJ, Menon DK, Stamatakis EA. Anything goes? Regulation of the neural processes underlying response inhibition in TBI patients. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2017; 27: 159-69.

 51.  Silberstein RB, Pipingas A, Farrow M, Levy F, Stough CK. Dopaminergic modulation of default mode network brain functional connectivity in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Brain Behav 2016; 6: e00582.

 52.  Mowinckel AM, Alnaes D, Pedersen ML, Ziegler S, Fredriksen M, Kaufmann T, et al. Increased default-mode variability is related to reduced task-performance and is evident in adults with ADHD. NeuroImage Clin 2017; 16: 369-82.

 53.  Matuskey A, Luo X, Zhang S, Morgan PT, Osama A, Malison R, et al. Methylphenidate remediates error-preceding activation of the default mode brain regions in cocaine-addicted individuals. Psychiatry Res 2013; 214: 116-21.

 54.  Battel L, Kieling RR, Kieling C, Anés M, Aurich NK, Da Costa JC, et al. Intrinsic brain connectivity following long-term treatment with methylphenidate in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 2016; 26: 555-61.

 55.  Uddin LQ, Kelly AMC, Biswal BB, Margulies DS, Shehzad Z, Shaw D, et al. Network homogeneity reveals decreased integrity of default-mode network in ADHD. J Neurosci Methods 2008; 169: 249-54.

 56.  Hart H, Radua J, Nakao T, Mataix-Cols D, Rubia K. Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: exploring task-specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70: 185-98.

 57.  Cortese S, Kelly C, Chabernaud C, Proal E, Di Martino A, Milham MP, et al. Toward systems neuroscience of ADHD: a meta-analysis of 55 fMRI studies. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169: 1038-55.

 58.  Hillary FG, Slocomb J, Hills EC, Fitzpatrick NM, Medaglia JD, Wang J, et al. Changes in resting connectivity during recovery from severe traumatic brain injury. Int J Psychophysiol 2011; 82: 115-23.

 59.  Pandit AS, Expert P, Lambiotte R, Bonnelle V, Leech R, Turkheimer FE. Traumatic brain injury impairs small-world topology. Neurology 2013; 80: 1826-33.

 60.  Venkatesan UM, Dennis NA, Hillary FG. Chronology and chronicity of altered resting-state functional connectivity after traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma 2015; 32: 252-64.

 61.  Bonnelle V, Ham TE, Leech R, Kinnunen KM, Mehta MA, Greenwood RJ, et al. Salience network integrity predicts default mode network function after traumatic brain injury. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2012; 109: 4690-5.

 62.  Li CS, Yan P, Bergquist KL, Sinha R. Greater activation of the ‘default’ brain regions predicts stop signal errors. Neuroimage 2007; 38: 640-8.

 63.  Castellanos FX, Proal E. Large-scale brain systems in ADHD: beyond the prefrontal-striatal model. Trends Cogn Sci 2012; 16: 17-26.

 64.  Mayer JS, Roebroeck A, Maurer K, Linden DE. Specialization in the default mode: task-induced brain deactivations dissociate between visual working memory and attention. Hum Brain Mapp 2010; 31: 126-39.

 65.  Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002; 3: 201-15.

 66.  Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen C, Raichle ME, et al. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005; 102: 9673-8.

 67.  Li CS, Morgan PT, Matuskey D, Abdelghany O, Luo X, Chang JL, et al. Biological markers of the effects of intravenous methyl-phenidate on improving inhibitory control in cocaine-dependent patients. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2010; 107: 14455-9.

 68.  Sofuoglu M. Cognitive enhancement as a pharmacotherapy target for stimulant addiction. Addiction 2010; 105: 38-48.

 69.  Vansickel AR, Fillmore MT, Hays LR, Rush CR. Effects of potential agonist-replacement therapies for stimulant dependence on inhibitory control in cocaine abusers. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 2008; 34: 293-305.

 70.  Moeller FG, Schmitz JM, Herin D, Kjome KL. Use of stimulants to treat cocaine and methamphetamine abuse. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2008; 10: 385-91.

 71.  Wenthur CJ. Classics in chemical neuroscience: methylphenidate. ACS Chem Neurosci 2016; 7: 1030-40.

 72.  Natsheh JY, Shiflett MW. The effects of methylphenidate on goal-directed behavior in a rat model of ADHD. Front Behav Neurosci 2015; 9: 326.

 73.  Kline RL, Zhang S, Farr OM, Hu S, Zaborszky L, Samanez-Larkin GR, et al. The effects of methylphenidate on resting-state functional connectivity of the basal nucleus of Meynert, locus coeruleus, and ventral tegmental area in healthy adults. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10: 149.

 74.  Aron AR, Dowson JH, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Methylphenidate improves response inhibition in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 54: 1465-8.

 75.  Rubia K, Alegria AA, Cubillo AI, Smith AB, Brammer MJ, Radua J. Effects of stimulants on brain function in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 76: 616-28.

 

¿Cómo afecta el metilfenidato al circuito de activación por defecto? Revisión sistemática

Introducción. El metilfenidato es un fármaco ampliamente usado como tratamiento del trastorno por déficit de atención/hiperactividad (TDAH) y otros trastornos neuropsiquiátricos. La dificultad para mantener la atención de forma prolongada y la deficiente ejecución de tareas que caracterizan a tales trastornos se han vinculado a la disfunción del circuito de activación por defecto –default mode network (DMN)­–, revelado en estudios de resonancia magnética funcional. En los individuos sanos, el DMN y la red orientada a tareas (task-positive network) presentan una relación inversa. Se ha planteado que el metilfenidato revertiría la escasa desactivación del DMN durante la ejecución de tareas que caracteriza a los trastornos de la atención y del control inhibitorio, normalización que a su vez mejoraría la ejecución de las tareas.

Pacientes y métodos. Con objeto de examinar la hipótesis de que este fármaco propicia tal desactivación, se llevó a cabo una revisión sistemática de la bibliografía.

Resultados. Doce estudios se incluyeron finalmente en la revisión. Para ello, debían haber medido los efectos de la administración del metilfenidato sobre la actividad del DMN. Once estudios mostraron indicios de mejora atribuible al metilfenidato en áreas cerebrales vinculadas a dicho circuito. Los resultados indican la normalización de los circuitos cerebrales en los pacientes con disfunción del DMN.

Conclusiones. Los hallazgos preliminares ofrecen indicios sólidos de que el metilfenidato mejora la disfunción del DMN presente en el TDAH y otros trastornos neuropsiquiátricos. Se precisan nuevos estudios que diluciden los pormenores de este efecto y mejoren la comprensión sobre los mecanismos de acción del metilfenidato.

Palabras clave. Circuito de activación por defecto. DMN. Estado de reposo. Metilfenidato. Neurociencia. Psicofarmacología. TDAH. Trastorno por déficit de atención/hiperactividad.

 

© 2019 Revista de Neurología

Si ya es un usuario registrado en Neurologia, introduzca sus datos de inicio de sesión.


Rellene los campos para registrarse en Neurologia.com y acceder a todos nuestros artículos de forma gratuita
Datos básicos
He leído y acepto la política de privacidad y el aviso legal
Seleccione la casilla si desea recibir el número quincenal de Revista de Neurología por correo electrónico. De forma quincenal se le mandará un correo con los títulos de los artículos publicados en Revista de Neurología.
Seleccione la casilla si desea recibir el boletín semanal de Revista de Neurología por correo electrónico. El boletín semanal es una selección de las noticias publicadas diariamente en Revista de Neurología.
Seleccione la casilla si desea recibir información general de neurologia.com (Entrevistas, nuevos cursos de formación, eventos, etc.)
Datos complementarios

Se os solicita los datos de redes para dar repercusión por estos medios a las publicaciones en las que usted participe.

En cumplimiento de la Ley 34/2002, de 11 de julio, de Servicios de la Sociedad de la Información y de Comercio Electrónico (LSSI-CE), Viguera Editores, S.L.U. se compromete a proteger la privacidad de sus datos personales y a no emplearlos para fines no éticos.

El usuario otorga su consentimiento al tratamiento automatizado de los datos incluidos en el formulario. Los datos facilitados se tratarán siempre con la máxima confidencialidad, salvaguardando su privacidad y con los límites que establecen las leyes vigentes en España, y nunca se cederán a personas ajenas a la organización.

Usted tiene derecho a rectificar sus datos personales en cualquier momento informándolo a secretaria@viguera.com. También se le informa de la posibilidad de ejercitar el derecho de cancelación de los datos personales comunicados.



¡CONVIÉRTASE EN USUARIO PREMIUM DE NEUROLOGIA.COM!

Además, por convertirte en usuario premium, recibirá las siguientes ventajas:

  • Plaza asegurada en todos nuestros Másteres (www.ineurocampus.com)
  • Descuento del 5% en los cursos de “Actualización en Neurología”, la FMC que estará disponible próximamente en la web.
  • Descarga gratuita en formato PDF dos de las obras con más éxito publicadas por Viguera Editores:
    • Oromotors Disorders in childhood (M. Roig-Quilis; L. Pennington)
    • Manual de Neuropsicología 2ª ed. (J. Tirapu-Ustárroz; M. Ríos-Lago; F. Maestú)

El precio para hacerse Premium durante el periodo de un año es de 5€, que podrá pagar a continuación a través de una pasarela de pago seguro con tarjeta de crédito, transferencia bancaria o PayPal:

QUIERO HACERME PREMIUM

No deseo hacerme premium


QUIERO MATRICULARME

No deseo matricularme